Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Self-Esteem in Online Dating

Profile image of Brenda Bernad

2020, Agora Psycho-Pragmatica

Loading Preview

Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

PDXScholar logo with slogan Access for All.

Home > ETDS > Dissertations and Theses > 889

Dissertations and Theses

Self-disclosure and self-efficacy in online dating.

Andrew Christopher España , Portland State University Follow

Portland State University. Department of Communication

First Advisor

Leslie Rill

Date of Publication

Summer 1-2-2013

Document Type

Degree name.

Master of Science (M.S.) in Communication

Communication

Online dating -- Research -- Social aspects, Self-efficacy -- Sex differences, Self-disclosure -- Sex differences, Self-esteem -- Sex differences

10.15760/etd.889

Physical Description

1 online resource (iii, 86 p.)

This study explores online dating by studying the relationship between self-disclosure and self-efficacy in an online dating environment. This research study examines the way self-disclosure, self-efficacy, self-esteem, and gender interrelate in an online dating environment. This study includes, but is not limited to, discussion of the type of correlation between self-efficacy and self-esteem, the relationship between self-disclosure and self-efficacy, and the differences between men's and women's self-disclosure in an online dating environment. From conducting this study, the researcher was able to determine that there is a statistically significant relationship between gender and how it relates to self-disclosure and self-efficacy. With the results from the study, the understanding of how different variables relate to online dating and romantic relationships has been taken one step further as it helps fill the gap in the literature.

In Copyright. URI: http://rightsstatements.org/vocab/InC/1.0/ This Item is protected by copyright and/or related rights. You are free to use this Item in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s).

Persistent Identifier

http://archives.pdx.edu/ds/psu/8858

Recommended Citation

España, Andrew Christopher, "Self-Disclosure and Self-Efficacy in Online Dating" (2013). Dissertations and Theses. Paper 889. https://doi.org/10.15760/etd.889

Since July 08, 2013

Included in

Gender, Race, Sexuality, and Ethnicity in Communication Commons , Interpersonal and Small Group Communication Commons , Social Media Commons

Advanced Search

  • Notify me via email or RSS
  • Featured Collections
  • All Authors
  • Schools & Colleges
  • Dissertations & Theses
  • PDXOpen Textbooks
  • Conferences
  • Collections
  • Disciplines
  • Faculty Expert Gallery
  • Submit Research
  • Faculty Profiles
  • Terms of Use
  • Feedback Form

Home | About | My Account | Accessibility Statement | Portland State University

Privacy Copyright

  • Social Software
  • Computer Science and Engineering
  • Computer and Society
  • Online Dating

Online dating: A motivated behavior during pandemic

  • December 2020

Mary Ann at Christian Medical College Vellore

  • Christian Medical College Vellore

Gunjan Joshi at King George's Medical University

  • King George's Medical University
  • This person is not on ResearchGate, or hasn't claimed this research yet.

Ishangi Mishra at Jain University

  • Jain University

Discover the world's research

  • 25+ million members
  • 160+ million publication pages
  • 2.3+ billion citations
  • Dalibor Petrović
  • Kristina Pejković

Hannah R Marston

  • Hochschule Fresenius
  • Chandra Shekar

Kashif Mansoor

  • Shari Krishnaratne
  • April Mazzuca

Denton Callander

  • Ryan Deveau
  • Dustin T. Duncan
  • David P. Richardson

Julie Anne Lovegrove

  • Patralekha Chatterjee
  • Jai Bhagwan Sharma
  • Eshani Sharma
  • Sangeeta Sharma
  • Janmeeta Singh

Alka Anand Subramanyam

  • Hrishikesh B. Nachane

Niraj N Mahajan

  • Chantelle Otten

Lauren M Walker

  • MATH BIOSCI

Calistus Ngonghala

  • Recruit researchers
  • Join for free
  • Login Email Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google Welcome back! Please log in. Email · Hint Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google No account? Sign up

Profiling the Self in Mobile Online Dating Apps: a Serial Picture Analysis

  • Arena of Identity
  • Open access
  • Published: 13 February 2021
  • Volume 6 , pages 147–171, ( 2023 )

Cite this article

You have full access to this open access article

online dating and self esteem research paper

  • Johanna Lisa Degen   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-0530-1385 1 &
  • Andrea Kleeberg-Niepage 1  

21k Accesses

7 Citations

33 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

Profiles in the widely used phenomenon of mobile online dating applications are characteristically reduced to condensed information mostly containing one or a few pictures. Thus, these picture(s) play a significant role for the decision-making processes and success, supposedly holding vital meaning for the subjects. While profile pictures in social media are omnipresent and some research has already focused on these pictures, especially selfies, there has been little attention with regards to the actual self-presentation when mobile online dating. In this paper, we show the results of a reconstructive serial analysis of 524 mobile online dating profile pictures investigating how subjects present themselves in the context of a mobile online dating app. This context is highly specific and characterized by continuous and dichotomous judgments by (unknown) others, unseen competition, and permanent validation of the self. Despite the conceivable multitude of possible self-presentations, our analysis led to eight clear types of self-presentation. Contemplating on subject’s good reasons for presenting the self as one of many and not as varied and unique when mobile online dating, we refer to the discourse of the private self (Gergen, The saturated self: Dilemmas of identity in contemporary life, Basic Books, New York, 1991; Rose, Governing the soul: Shaping of the private self, Free Association Books, London, 2006) and to (Holzkamp, 1983. Grundlagen der Psychologie. Frankfurt a.M.: Campus.) concept of restrictive and generalized agency in a context of socially constituted norms.

Similar content being viewed by others

online dating and self esteem research paper

The More We Tinder: Subjects, Selves and Society

online dating and self esteem research paper

It’s not You, it’s Me: Experiences with Ghosting on Mobile Dating Applications and Belgian Emerging Adults’ Self-Esteem

Personal preferences, discursive strategies, and the maintenance of inequality on gay dating apps, explore related subjects.

  • Artificial Intelligence

Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.

Finding a Match Based on a Single Picture: the Characteristics of Online Mobile Dating

What once was a dress, a suit, the perfume, make up, the car to arrive in, shoes, body language—embodied knowledge and the first impression—is now captured in one picture when mobile online dating. Today, the dating reality of millions of users comes in the form of digital dating applications on mobile devices characterized by portability, availability, locatability, and multimediality (Schrock, 2015 ). These deliver (nonstop) algorithm-based suggestions of possible partners with the obvious effects of a multiplication of options and acceleration of the search for a partner, as well as a specific frame for the self-presentation, namely the individual profile picture. This is the current stage of a long history of changes in dating practices and the establishment of intimate relationships: once it was the arrangement of a marriage, suggested spouses by the social circle like family and friends, it then became more a question of coincidence in terms of, do I meet a spouse in everyday life? The former patterns were mostly local and social bound (Weigel, 2016 )—one met who was in reach, a crucial characteristic also of former analogue matchmaking services. Online dating agencies then overcame the characteristic of the locally bound but sharing the characteristic of matchmaking based on rich and complex information. Online dating websites included extensive psychological questionnaires to pair the right and presumably matching individuals. Here the suggestion of a partner is detached from friends and family and their expertise, to the ‘expertise’ of shared lifestyle and onto a calculating tool, still striving to match on the basis of personal and detailed information about each individual. Matching partners due to professionalized websites, was the first digital example to overcome distance and to detach from both social surrounding, everyday practices and geographical position—one could find a partner on a national or even international basis broadening the dating pool. Today, however, dating is increasingly practiced as mobile online dating via applications, also known as local-bound-online- or microdating, with two main aspects that have changed significantly. First, the dating apps returned to locally bound principles fueled by GPS, suggesting a spouse geographically nearby and physically accessible. Second, the apps operate with limited condensed information, as only a minimum amount of information like age, sexual orientation and gender is required and decision making then is based on mainly one or a few pictures.

Based on such scarce information the app works as a matchmaker and expert, suggesting partners based on the following criteria: physical location, age, gender, and sexual orientation. Even though profile texts of up to 500 characters are possible, research shows that the decision whether to like or dislike a profile is mainly based on the impression of the first profile picture (Ward, 2016 ). Profile texts function mostly as an appendix and are often not considered or read at all (Degen & Kleeberg-Niepage, 2020 ). This lack of engagement with profile texts can be explained by the accelerated and dynamic practice of swiping along (ibd.). This does not deny a significance of the profile texts, yet the profile-pictures play a predominant and crucial role in the practice of mobile online dating. Profiles of proposed partners—typically the first picture allows the first impression of the suggested other—respectively give the other an impression of oneself. This might sound similar to analogue dating, like meeting in a club dancing, but unlike analogue settings, the picture is a static and scenic construction of the self. The presenter chooses motif, image section and perspective, selects one specific picture for the specific context. Moreover, the presenter has the options to edit, an intentional process of presenting the self in terms of optimization, success, and reacting to an (assumed) external feedback.

Despite the obvious importance of profile pictures in online dating apps and the general fact that pictures play a (more and more) important role in subjects’ lives and experiences, psychological research so far does not address the significance of (these) pictures, but largely ignores them as independent data (Reavey, 2016 ). To bridge this gap and to explore how subjects visually present the self in the context of mobile an online dating, we conducted a qualitative serial analysis of 542 Tinder profile pictures.

In this paper, we will first shortly review the state of research in the field of mobile online dating and in profile pictures and discuss significant theoretical perspectives to grasp the specifics of visually presenting the self in this context. Subsequently, we introduce the methodological framework and methodical procedure of our qualitative serial analyses of profile pictures. Then we present the typology that emerged from the analysis and discuss these results referring to (a) the discourse of the private self (Gergen, 1991 ; Rose, 2006 ) and (b) to Holzkamp’s ( 1983 ) concept of restrictive and generalized agency in a context of socially constituted norms. With the hypotheses derived from the serial analysis, we finally give an outlook on the upcoming single picture analyses and future research perspectives in general.

Research on Mobile Online Dating and the Profile Pictures

There is a growing amount of interdisciplinary research of the social sciences on online dating applications. This research comes from different disciplines, most prominently the field of media and communication, with valuable perspectives (e.g., understanding a selfie in regard to sexual orientation, analyzing the development of codes and norms in communication, usage of emojicons and so forth, i.a. Highfield & Leaver, 2016 ; Duguay, 2016a , b ; Courtois & Timmermans, 2018 ). Psychological research has so far focused on personal traits and behavior regarding the degree of honesty when presenting the self or on specific traits and personality characteristics of dating-app users (Charpenter & McEwan, 2016 ; Musil et al., 2017 ), user’s motivations (Timmermans & DeCaluwe, 2017 ; Ward, 2016 ) or the extent of disclosure (e.g., their face) (Fitzpatrick et al., 2015 ), accuracy of profile (-pictures) (Duguay, 2016a , b ; Hancock & Toma, 2007 ) or potentially hidden information in the profile pictures in the form of conclusions about ones personality based on the presentation (Ward, 2016 ).

While there is some research about profile pictures (e.g., on Facebook), little research regards profile pictures in the specific context of mobile online dating. The little research deals with profile pictures in the context of mobile online dating specifically subdivides into qualitative and quantitative approaches and their respective perspectives.

Research following a quantitative, structural perspective demonstrates general behavior and the correlation of profiles and personal traits. Ranzini and Lutz ( 2017 ) combine location-based real-time dating and personal traits and picture analysis in regard to narcissism, loneliness, and self-esteem with motives and real or deceptive self-presentation. They show that realistic, that is more authentic, presentation tends to correlate positively with friendship-seeking motives and negatively with validation motives. Authenticity is higher when motives are relational and lower when having lower self-esteem, which also is correlated positively with sexual orientation. Sedgewick et al. ( 2017 ) found that the angle of selfies is gender related, men taking pictures from below and women from above. Another large quantitative analysis of 22 million online dating profile pictures show that younger men often show up in sporting activities, with animals or weapons and older men would display luxurious accessories like boats, motorbikes or champagne. Younger women, on the other hand, more frequently show off their bodies (e.g., by showing themselves in swimwear), place themselves in attractive or exotic landscapes, while older women would also display luxury goods like designer clothes (cf. Pleines et al., 2017 ). This research perspective mostly concerns assumptions about the intentions of the users which have been derived from the photographic representations whereby the photo has mainly been used as an illustration of (supposedly) inherent motifs (e.g., with wearing a swimsuit as a signal for the readiness for sex) (ibid).

The qualitative perspective mainly focuses on subjects meaning making, intentions and behavior when creating profiles. Ellison emphasizes the specific role of profile pictures when online dating: “Profiles are essential for online daters because they constitute a gateway for future FtF (face to face) dating” ( 2012 , p. 2). She continues that it is specific for mobile online dating that decisions about a possible romantic partner are made within seconds, which leads to concerns about missing good options. The self-presentation furthermore is characterized as cue-reduced, as a process of selective, fragmentized and condensed self-presentation, where the information one gives about oneself is “malleable and subject to self-censorship” (Ellison, 2012 , p. 3).

As mobile online dating is a process of asynchronous communication, it changes the general aspects of the presentation of the self, as it creates a time lag with possibilities to edit and modify, which also results in a tension between being attractive and honest (Walther, 1996 ,  2007 ). One can thoroughly create and optimize a profile and pictures, meaning the self-presentation is a planned enactment with a decisive time advantage, but this leaves the user with the question to what degree the self should be optimized to avoid insecurity and disappointment when meeting in real life. These characteristics result in plenty of research about honesty, deception, and authenticity and following rules and acceptance. Some degree of optimization seems collectively acceptable, for instance small optimization of professional status. In contrast, other deviations between analogue person and digital profile is highly condemned, for instance the body type or hair length (Degen & Kleeberg-Niepage, 2020 ). The acceptance of the (supposedly little) lies or rather optimizations are not always interpreted as lack of authenticity, when they are intentionally and realistically obtainable in the future, they are instead mutually accepted (Ellison et al., 2006 ).

Thus, the state of research only entails work on optimization and modification or raises questions about intentions and perceptions of self-presentation so far. A reconstructive perspective on series of profile pictures accessing the picture’s inherent logic has not been carried out. Such an approach can contribute to explicate socio-cultural storylines as frames of orientation, norms, and usual practices and thus the presenters’ often implicit knowledge on rules and standards of self-presentation in the context of online dating.

The Self and Its Profile Picture

Images are omnipresent and constantly gain importance in digital realities, social media, profiles, and documentation of everyday practices (Reavey, 2016 ). In this context, the self-presentation of subjects becomes more and more important either as a means of recognizability (like a Facebook profile picture), or to tell stories about one’s life or current activities (Instagram, WhatsApp status).

For mobile online dating, profile pictures are even more important than in other social media because decisions of like or dislike are mainly based on one picture representing “it all” or rather “what’s recognizable and likable” (Ward, 2016 ).

In these digital dating contexts, subjects seek and receive feedback and evaluation regarding their visual self-representations. Although this might be similar to analogue encounters—as social feedback is a vital source for self-related cognitions and emotions (Gergen, 1991 ; James, 1890 ; Mummendey, 2006 ; Harter, 2012 )—in digital contexts feedback and evaluation is only directed to a two-dimensional representation of the person, not to the person as a whole. Nevertheless, these judgments are experienced by the whole person influencing self-perception and self-esteem and thereby the way the self is presented in the future. As a kind of self-protection many social media profiles can be turned to a private-mode enabling users to select carefully who is able to see and comment on one’s pictures.

In mobile online dating, however, this possibility of self-protection is no longer there. Creating a profile means to open up to a wider public, to an assembly of unknown others, at least within the range of the predefined criteria. Furthermore, the context of online dating with its aims of finding an intimate partner—for either one night or for the rest of one’s life—leaves subjects in an especially vulnerable position with negative evaluation resulting in a threat to the self. Additionally, compared to other social media, negative evaluation within mobile online dating apps is rather implicit for one will never know how many people looked at one’s profile without liking it and profiles cannot simply be commented on negatively. Therefore, knowledge about one’s success or failure can only be acquired by comparing one’s likes, matches and dates with others (e.g., friends or colleagues) developing a competitive logic.

Of course, comparison and competition with others is an inevitable source of self-related cognitions, emotions and behavior, of self-confidence, self-perception, and self-esteem (Festinger, 1954 ; Morse & Gergen, 1970 ) with positive evaluation pushing and negative feedback decreasing it. Nevertheless, the self-presentation in an online dating profile, being reduced from a complex process of verbal, para- and nonverbal behavior and activities (Goffman, 1959 ) to a few static characteristics, leaves the subject with limited options to prevent the self from setbacks. When one comes off worse in such a competition (i.e., having few matches or dates), subjects—besides optionally leaving the app or lowering their expectations—change and thereby supposedly optimize the profile (picture) to increase success. For the optimization subjects then occasionally reach out for help by friends or colleagues and preferable those being more successful, copying profile styles and texts or even turn to professional tutorials and coaching (Illouz, 2007 ). Such optimization of one’s self-presentation is reminiscent of what Rose ( 2006 ) and Gergen ( 1991 ) call the discourse of the private self with subjects taking responsibility for previous failures, internalizing them into the self.

However, for one’s self-presentation subjects must largely refer to an unknown and instead imagined audience with few cues to rely on (Goffman, 1959 , 1981 ; Litt, 2012 ). To please this imagined audience users have to relate to—often rather implicit—norms of self-presentation they expect to be successful in an online dating context. When relating to these norms, subjects have at least two options: one option would be to follow and submit to the abovementioned competitive logic of success and adapt one’s self-presentation accordingly, exercising what Holzkamp ( 1983 ) called restrictive agency. A second option would be to avoid a quantitative logic of comparison and competition or even consciously reject competition at all, creating a highly specific profile and e.g. emphasize other criteria of success like the intensiveness of the dates. Such activities would provide a more sovereign subject position and maybe come close to what Holzkamp (ibid.) called generalized agency.

These theoretical considerations provide the framework for our serial analysis of profile pictures and for responding to the following questions:

How do subjects shape—construct and reconstruct—the self in the specific context of dating app profiles (accelerated daily practice, continuous judgment by others, unseen competition, validation of the self, exposure, self-esteem), in the tension between authenticity, exposure, and subjectivity on the one hand and the implied social norms and disclosure to unknown others on the other hand? How do subjects position themselves towards these norms in terms of subjective sovereignty, conformity, and moments of resistance?

Methodological Framework and Methodical Procedure

A reconstructive perspective on pictures as social practice.

Our research question is aimed at the meaning making processes involved when subjects visually present the self in a mobile online dating app profile. This focus and the fact, that an analysis of such pictures is a still novel and rather explorative endeavor calls for a qualitative procedure which is targeted at a deeper understanding of subjects’ sense-making procedures and the development of new presumptions. Reconstructive methods follow specific quality criteria—transparency, intersubjectivity, scope—that differ from those of quantitative research (Steinke, 2010 ). These are based on the epistemological assumption that reality does not exist independently of the observer but is constructed in a creative and reciprocal process between empirical data and researcher. Because of these particularities, replication would be neither possible nor useful; we therefore place particular emphasis on the transparency of our approach in the following sections.

Instead of asking people for their interpretation of the pictures and thereby producing a narration about pictures, we have analyzed the pictures as such. In doing this, we consider pictures as social practice and interactive behavior (Pilarczyk & Mietzner, 2008 ) and as expression of subjects’ (often implicit) knowledge of the social world and their own position in this world. We therefore assume, that (an analysis of) pictures will provide access to people’s ways of relating the self to this world and thereby their (perceived) scope of action.

Such an understanding of pictures corresponds to the reconstructive research paradigm within which specifically Documentary Method developed a picture-analytical procedure to reveal the creators implicit (or atheoretical) knowledge contained in the pictures (e.g., Bohnsack, 2010 ; Bohnsack et al., 2015 ). Nevertheless, picture analyses within the reconstructive research paradigm almost exclusively focus on single pictures or on small series (e.g., Michel, 2015 ) whereas larger samples or series of pictures are rarely addressed.

For our purpose, we relied on and combined the only two approaches (at least to our knowledge) that are reconstructive and dedicated to large datasets—serial analysis by Mietzner and Pilarczyk ( 2008 ) and first impression analysis (Ersteindrucksanalyse) by Müller-Dohm ( 1997 ). Both differ in their procedure and the ascribed meaning of the serial analysis for the research project. For Müller-Dohm, first impression analysis is a preliminary step that precedes the actual detailed case analysis: “In order to achieve a first systematization of the […] material, it is extremely helpful to carry out so-called first impression analyses of all cases prior to the actual individual case analysis” (p. 102, translated by the authors). In contrast, for Mietzner and Pilarczyk both serial and single picture analysis are equally significant and do not have a strict order: “The aim of serial analyses is to detect continuous or discontinuous developments, anomalies and differences as well as to classify and interpret them.” (p. 142, translated by the authors). Such an analysis “offers the chance to recognize and interpret the enforcement of image patterns and styles” (ibid.) and enables to understand and detect specifics, which is impossible when exclusively studying single cases.

Despite these differences both approaches share with Documentary Method the reference to art-historical procedures, especially to the work of Erwin Panofsky ( 1955 ), who at his lifetime changed art-historical picture analysis fundamentally, namely from iconography to iconology. Panofsky understood a picture as a document of an epoch or of the personality of the artist and developed a three-step method to access the picture’s inner structure. His three-step out of pre-iconographic denotation, iconographic interpretation, and iconological analysis is often described as the shift in analytical attitude from the what to the how (Bohnsack,  2010 ).

Panofsky's ( 1975 ) approach and methodological steps were demonstrated by a simple scene, the pulling of hats, when two people (in the early twentieth century) meet on the street.

Step one (pre-iconographic denotation) of Panofsky’s iconology describes the strictly formal appearance that excludes all interpretative knowledge and assumptions as well as cultural meanings. In our scene, this corresponds to the statement of a gesture—a person lifts his hat as he passes.

Step two (iconographic interpretation) approaches the meaning of the scene by assuming so-called um-zu motifs (Bohnsack, 2009 , p. 30), in our scene, for example: A gentleman lifts his hat to greet.

Step three (iconological analysis) opens up the historical and socio-cultural meaning of the scene and considers the gesture of pulling one's hat as a class-, gender-, culture-specific practice. This is understood by the other only if he shares his space of experience.

By means of this procedure, an image can be grasped in detail and with the bracketing of one's own prior knowledge, and analyzed for all possible meanings in the respective cultural context.

Data Collection and Analytical Procedure

Data collection and sample.

We collected profile pictures, a total of 542 heterosexual male and female profiles which we gathered via two specially created heterosexual profiles (one female, one male) in a northern German city. We chose Tinder as one example of a local bound mobile dating application for two main reasons. First, it is currently the most popular application in Germany and worldwide with 56 million users (Statista, 2020 ; Ward, 2016 ). Second, earlier research showed that, specifically in this application, the pictures play a significant role when deciding on possible partners, with other details such as the profile text playing a minor role. We collected the data on a day in November 2017 between 6 and 11 pm (242 profiles) and a day in May 2020 (same time and same location with 300 profiles). The targeted age group was 25–33 years old. We switched between male and female accounts every half hour to make the sample as equal as possible regarding external impact.

Procedure of the Serial Picture Analysis

For the concrete procedure of analyzing the whole series Müller-Dohm ( 1997 ) suggests three steps. First, the description of the first impression of the picture, i.e. the primary message, objects and persons that are depicted, stylistic elements, and primary presentation; second, the preliminary formation of types based on similarities; and third, the formation of types including the selection of a prototype. Mietzner and Pilarczyk ( 2008 ) who consider a sample of 300 to 500 pictures as suitable for a serial analysis propose different strategies like arranging series of motives to register changes (e.g., over time, to record the elements of the picture design, common motives or topics) to eventually create types as well. While for Müller-Dohm the term 'type' denotes similarities on the level of motifs, Mietzner and Pilarczyk go beyond this and ask about the socio-historical significance of frequently appearing themes, motifs, and modes of representation (ibid. p. 143f). For our analysis, we combined these suggested procedures while conducting a three-step procedure as well.

As a first step, we split into two groups and described the comprehensive samples motifs, strictly descriptive to get an understanding of the profile presentation.

We described the profiles regarding the person visualized in terms of perspective/angle/body parts, gaze, gesture, posture, mimic, dressing, objects used, background, distance from camera, background, items, and other subjects and compared the results. This procedure is close to the first step suggested by Müller-Dohm ( 1997 ) and corresponds to Panofsky’s ( 1955 ) pre-iconographic description.

Example: Subject/person, full body posture, showing the face including the eyes, palm tree in the background, wearing a light/short dress and a hat with a prominent badge on the front and a backpack—no requisites, no other people, no animals.

General sample description: The comprehensive sample shows that self-presentation on Tinder is dominated by images arranged around one person in the images’ center. The self-presentation can differ in terms of angles, backgrounds and props or objects, variation and degree of filter, yet there are some overall characteristics for certain kinds of pictures.

First there are motives with the person predominantly filling the image, displaying mostly the face in a close up, possibly displaying the bust or torso, with hair framing the face, sometimes little gestures with the hands or small objects like jewelry, glasses or pets, only little background insights without explicit indications for actions or interests are given. With this type, the background remains a secondary motive with little information. Typical backgrounds are nature surroundings, urban locations or interior/indoor locations.

The second general class of pictures displays the whole body/posture often supplemented by a background playing a larger role for the scene created. Here the background is part of a setting, often the person is using the background to demonstrate an action, interest or lifestyle and (also larger) objects like sports equipment are presented. Backgrounds show travel scenery and exotic landscapes or animals, specific surroundings like sports facilities, harbors or airports, urban architecture or statues and art in the form of street art or sculptures, bars/restaurants or clubs. These pictures can also contain other people, showing one other person or the subject among several others, posing for the picture or while engaged in an activity.

The third kind of self-presentation comes as pictures only showing parts of the body or the face, or the back of a person, or display objects or props to stay incognito like helmets, sunglasses, or umbrellas. Some single examples show no person at all but objects or avatars.

Based on a random sample of 140 pictures, we then arranged, rearranged, and compared the pictures following similarities and discrepancies according to Mietzner and Pilarczyk ( 2008 ), forming a kind of preliminary typology (according to Müller-Dohms second step, 1997 ). As we referred to common sense knowledge and external sources during this step, it corresponds to Panofsky’s ( 1955 ) iconographic interpretation.

Complete image of a young woman presenting herself against the background of an exotic landscape in summer clothing plus a hat with an Australian flag—refers to summer temperatures and stay abroad (often associated with vacation, leisure, travel, far from everyday life).

Representation of a male subject in a beach scenery, wearing a wetsuit, and holding a surfboard. Displaying a situation of leisure activity, hobbies in a non-everyday practices’ environment, possibly holidays.

Both descriptions include information about activity, habits and lifestyle or taste and are in the final typology collected under the type “informative.”

With the last step, we confronted our preliminary typology with the comprehensive sample, extended or modified it where necessary and eventually developed our typology (based on Müller-Dohm, 1997 and Mietzner & Pilarczyk, 2008 ).

Saturation was reached with less than two hundred pictures. The great majority fit clearly into the typology; this also counts for the sample from 2020. We contemplate on the meaning of each type and subsequently situate the final typology within theoretical and empirical insights and discuss the meaning of our results for both subjects’ perspectives and their behavior with regards to social norms. This step parallels Panofsky’s ( 1955 ) iconological analysis.

The Tinder Typology of Self-presentation: Being One of Many—countless Opportunities, Few Types

Our reconstructive serial picture analysis resulted in a typology with eight types of different magnitude (Table 1 ).

Nevertheless, a bigger seize of a type does not necessarily equate to greater significance which can be seen from the following detailed presentation of the individual types. Please note: For anonymity, the picture depicted are reduced to a sketch and blurred in regard to background and details and furthermore censored in regard to the eyes and parts of the face, age and nickname. Thus, we assume that no harm for the user can occur from our publication (for further ethical considerations see “ Ethics and Limitations ” section).

The selfie type, characterized by self-control and limits:

figure a

Description: A selfie is a picture taken by the person him- or herself where the angle, the proportion of head, shoulder, and arm show that photographer and photographed person are the same subject. While not hiding the fact of taking the picture him- or herself differences occur in posing, angle, person-camera relation, objects and gestures. There are different kinds of selfies coming with “direct eye contact,” where the subject looks into the camera,” “indirect eye contact,” where the person is watching the self on the screen, “showing skin” where subjects are (seemingly) partly naked, “no eye contact” where subjects seemingly look away or have closed eyes, look towards the ground, and “mirror selfies,” where the picture is taken in front of a mirror often showing the device and selfie “with context reference,” where the picture contains explicit information about the surrounding, for instance one person taking a selfie with an elephant.

Meaning: Taking a selfie is a moment and an act with the self. It makes obsolete the seemingly natural difference of photographer and photographed. On the one hand, the selfie is an act of control and full subjective agency; on the other hand, the act and the picture are restricting the objective to armlength and angle. This means the resulting picture is including the typical and explicit characteristics, e.g. showing the prominent shoulder. For subjects, taking a selfie has some advantages. First and foremost, they are controlling the moment of taking the photograph. Second compared to pictures taken from other, it secures the privacy of not socially exposing the enaction of looking good and ‘wanting’ a carefully orchestrated close-up and often multiple attempts striving for the perfect scene, angle, light and so forth—characteristics often perceived as unpleasant or awkward. Furthermore, it comes with the advantage of immediate availability and the spontaneous capturing of a moment where the person felt well or even happy or wants to capture a situation.

The informative type, characterized by lifestyle and romantic echoes:

figure b

Description: Pictures of this type give insights into hobbies/activities/lifestyle by showing them in the background. The person here is either posing with for instance, sports equipment, in front of travel sceneries, when commuting, with a pet or an animal or in surroundings, for instance, a yacht club for eating dinner, golf course eco-friendly/fair-trade coffee places, or props and objects for sexual practices like whips or costumes (e.g., cosplay).

Meaning: The informative picture goes beyond the selfie in several ways. It is socially interactional because someone else is taking the photograph. At the same time, including cues from the background the motif leads to some distance between photographer and photographed person, sometimes the photographed persons face is hard to recognize. Even though it centers one subject, it gives insights/information about the background, possible hobbies, interests, an activity or specific experience or skills/capabilities and is thus hinting to a certain lifestyle. This can function as a correspondence to the romantic ideal of relationships, which contains aspects of exotic contexts, specialties, and consumption. These self-presentations invite the viewer not only to see the subjects face but function as an invitation to a possible lifestyle, a certain scenery to join, an idea of how it might be to be with the person and the course of life of an possible future. These pictures are presenting some extraordinary activity, the opposite of everyday life and responsibilities, profiling the subject as active, exceptional, international and with stamina. The presentation is an invitation to personal information or to a possible lifestyle and personal capabilities.

The snapshot type, characterized by a situated moment:

figure c

Description: Snapshots are pictures that seem to be taken without the photographed person being conscious of it, one is presented in a seemingly natural situation or engaged in an activity, e.g. giving a speech, walking a street and shopping, doing sports, simply being in action and explicitly distracted from the situation of being photographed. There occur several forms of presentation, the portrait, where the subject is close to the camera and the image shows facial features and facial expressions and only the torso is photographed. Further forms are “in motion” where the person is walking, cooking, or playing a game and literally moving in the moment the picture is taken and “posing,” which does not address posing for the camera but more posing in a context, often these are males holding a beer and a cigarette, legs spread in an urban scenery.

figure d

Meaning: The subject is in the center of the picture, where the face and facial (micro-) expression is recognizable and takes the audience into the atmosphere of the moment capturing and communicating the aura of the photographed subject. The distance is generally not as close as the selfie but closer than the informative and social types, and the context plays a minor role. The photograph is created in two subjects’ social interaction, whereby the photographed subject seemingly does not notice the picture being taken in that moment or at least they are not explicitly posing, it thus references a form of authenticity. The photographer is capturing a specific moment, where the subjects seem to have a moment worthy of being captured. This implicitly points to the social relation where the photographer is willing and focused on capturing a moment centering the other; thus, these pictures contrast with the selfie.

The type of sociability and enjoyment, characterized by cues to network and milieu:

figure e

Description: This type shows one central person in a surrounding with other subjects, either as a group interacting or in a social context for instance a club in the form of a “party.” Often these images are arranged in context of food and drinks, smoking equipment, costumes and ceremonies like weddings.

Meaning: In comparison to the other types here there is more than one subject centered, often it is unclear which one is the profiles owner. These pictures also indicate some background, taste and activity, but moreover point to sociality, possible social competence, a lack of loneliness and profiles the subject as entertaining and as being a person worth being around. This presentation hints to a lifestyle of being social and around others, localizing the subject as being part of a bigger network/group and social contexts. This context, in some examples, references social responsibility in the form of children or elderly. These pictures are similar to the informative type giving insights into lifestyle and a certain course of life as a possible partner. In particular, this type gives insights into certain tastes and milieu, often these pictures are appear quite spontaneous, with little arrangement and (perhaps unintendingly) reflect places and interests (in the form of for instance sky bars, expensive hotels and clubs or shisha bars or a certain style of vacation).

The type professional photoshoot, characterized by an orchestrated self:

figure f

Description: These are pictures that seem to be taken by a professional photographer, meaning there is a clearly arranged set, lighting, and a high-quality image. A person here is posing for the picture. A specific way of presentation is the “CV/Resume picture”, which looks like a picture usually used for job applications or a LinkedIn profile, often the person is dressed quite formally (wearing a suit or a blouse).

Meaning: These types are special in terms of the photographer and photographed relationship as it is grounded in a an often paid and one-sided service. The centering of the photographed subject is based on this specific contract not on a social bond or intimacy, but the opposite as the photographer in most cases is a stranger conducting her or his profession as a service. The assignment is to focus on a specific look or feature (beauty, erotic, fitness, job application and so forth), thus focusing on themes and enhancing the emphasized characteristics and communicating the photographer’s general style as an artist, then the specific photographed subject. Furthermore, the situation is characteristically set-in scene/orchestrated by posing, light installation, and prerequisites such as specific clothes. The logic of the professional photograph is counterintuitive to the non-committal accelerated logic of low investment predominantly seen when mobile online dating as it is an explicit investment.

The incognito type, characterizing a hide and seek:

figure g

Description: This type of picture shows a person who is hiding or not showing their face. Either with an object like a hat, the cellphone, a hand, or an umbrella in front of the face, by showing the back, by only showing body parts or parts of the face or are taken from a distance that the subject is unrecognizable. Other present a tilted head or, mostly women, cover their face with hair either because the wind blows or demonstratively use it to cover the face.

Meaning: This type contains pictures of mirror selfies and selfies but also pictures taken from others, for instance where the subject is photographed from behind. The main characteristic is that the persons face is hidden and thus the subject stays incognito. Hiding here has many facets, taking mirror selfies hiding behind the device, covering the face with one hand, jewelry, a hat or with their own hair or choosing a picture taken from behind. Often these photos relocate the focus from the face or the whole person to the body or body parts like the lips or eyes and so forth. Being incognito and protecting the identity has meaning on several levels. First, judgment by the unknown other is relativized. Second, the logic of being presented and available is countered. Instead, the incognito picture reveals a political, normative or social stand and creates an artificial hurdle and exclusivity. The other has to dare the like, invest in the relationship and qualify for the reveal of the subject in terms of identity and look. This pattern constructs a sphere of mystique, a tension of the play with curiosity, control and what might hide behind the mask.

The type suspending the subject, characterized by seduction to projection:

figure h

Description: A small number of pictures could not be classified following the typology shown above. These all hide the identity and furthermore the body of the account holder and instead an avatar, objects or animals are shown. One picture showed a fictitious character from a comic, two pictures showed objects like trash, one showed a bird, one a single flower.

Meaning: The suspension of the subject is countering the logic of quick decisions based on seeing the possible partner. Similar to the type incognito the subjects create a (even stronger) sphere of hiding, curiosity, the dare to like at a possible future reveal. Here, there are no hints to the subjects looks, body type or features and furthermore the presented objects invite a huge range of interpretation, speculation, and projection—what does the audience understand from seeing a bird or trash? Is it saying something about the platform, the other or the self or neither of those? Leaving the other with the responsibility of what is seen and the decision whether to like or not.

Type: Challenging the logic, characterized by being counter habitual:

figure i

Description: Subversion of habitual self-presentation rarely occurred. Single exceptions included the overweight or subjects with children, or the use of irony to create distance from the medium and societal behavior regarding gender/dating stereotypes. In these pictures symbols or actions are presented but not exercised that in both cases address the stereotypical presumptions about the male provider role. In two cases the person is half naked, having a cigar or a cigarette in his mouth which is not lit, holding play money.

Meaning: These presentations stick out in number and logic in comparison to the other types. Here the subjects refer to the social expectations revealing either personal information about responsibilities, everyday life and restrictions, like having children or being handicapped or take a personal stand in the form of distancing the self from norms and gender-roles by using irony and exaggeration or challenging habitual aesthetics. Subjects thus invite viewers into a specific lifestyle, milieu or political stand and intellectual capability (humor) for a communicative relationship and positioning in a rather provocative/ polarizing way.

All types may occur with optional cross-sectional criteria like hats, different types of clothes, gestures and filters, such as color schemes. Filters and color schemes vary in intensity, from slight manipulations of the skin texture up to drastic blurs almost beyond recognition. These pictures come as overexposed or beauty filters are used, so that facial expressions and skin texture seem mask-like and artificial. A tension in picture editing between seemingly small edits, or artsy and explicit playing with graphical tools and drastically optimizing/editing occurs—for instance with snap chat filters, making the face cat-like or adding artificial graphical extras like birds, tattoos, freckles, and so forth.

New 2020 intersectional criterion: COVID-19 mask.

figure j

Given the situation of COVID-19, we were cautious to conclude about tendencies and comparison. A preliminary comparison shows a significant rise in selfies by up to almost 50%, many more sunglasses covering the face are presented (which probably correlates with seasons). Furthermore, there is a rise in pictures with accompanying subjects and there is no picture in the type of irony. There are similarly many pictures taken outside and inside, just as many incognito, alongside one new cross-sectional criteria: the mouth and nose (COVID-19) masks. This leads to speculation about whether the increase of selfies might be responsive to the pandemic and isolation or a collective tendency, which will be disclosed with further data series.

Profile Pictures (in Mobile Online Dating Apps) at the Intersection Between Social Norms and Subjective Sovereignty

According to the logic of the reconstructive research paradigm, not everything is consciously enacted, but nothing is randomly created. Based on our results we can infer that subjects (visual) self-presentation in mobile online dating apps relies on implicit knowledge about social norms, unwritten rules and (assumed) expectations of an anonymous, imagined audience. These implicit rules, expectations and audiences apparently require highly controlled (selfie type) or staged (professional photoshoot type) representations, references to romantic ideals (informative type) or the playful nature of love (incognito type), the significance of authenticity (snapshot type) or one’s affability (sociability type).

As the vast majority of the profile pictures in our sample belongs to one of these types. Across almost all of them, we see rather similar pictures that comply with general viewing habits, showing subjects in habitual motifs. Considering online dating as a specific social arena, Tinder predominantly constitutes—at least visually—a sphere of pleasure and leisure, beyond conventional everyday life with its responsibilities or let alone challenging circumstances like having children or being handicapped.

The prescribed logic of self-presentation in online dating app profiles seems to reduce the uniqueness of the subject to a uniqueness of many. Subjects seem rather to strive for being easy to categorize and decide on within seconds—thus exercising restrictive agency in Holzkamp's ( 1983 ) sense—than presenting the self as different, unique or challenging. In the tension between commonness and uniqueness this complies to the rather quantitative logic of being likable by many and compliant to the other’s practices. Choosing one of few characters or roles investing in an optimized and compliant impression management (Goffman, 1959 ) might reflect that in the practice of swiping along conformity seems rewarded by getting likes and matches. One reason for this is possibly the perceived time pressure limiting the time for reflecting about the reasons for one’s decisions, where non-conformity could be disturbing and is rejected in an accelerated dynamic process. In consequence showing one’s uniqueness seems to compete with being likeable.

Conformity assumingly pays off also in terms of self-validation, with likes being the relevant currency; users strive for rewards by presenting themselves in certain successful ways – which then reduces the potential multitude of options to display oneself. This is reasonable as presenting the self on the app exposes a person to a liberal logic in Roses terms ( 2006 ). Failure then is counted as one’s own fault and personal shortcoming. Failing in this context might be rejection in the form of relatively few matches, getting unmatched, low answering rate or number of physical dates. These rejections then are attributed to not being either good enough at using the app, having a repelling profile or even worse to being not good enough as a person or at least not good enough in comparison to others (Gergen, 1991 ; Festinger, 1954 ). This again has, in Gergens understanding of the self, a negative impact on both self-esteem and self-worth.

In sum, inner psychological processes, such as validation and adjustment of the self, in this specific online-dating context is thus negotiated with imagined others, possibly idealized others creating a digital projective surface for supposedly social yet mostly projective self-processes.

Few users resist the omnipresent restrictive logic with pictures disrupting the common ways of self-presentation. These exceptions contain different qualities, firstly by suspending the subject, inviting into a space for interpretation and projection, directing the other into reflective processes about norms and desires. Second, by a counter-habitual self-presentation for instance by creating distance through reflection and irony Here, subjects undermine the logic of immediate attractiveness and easily categorizable self-presentation—for instance, a guy sitting on the toilet or a guy posing in bed with an unlit cigar while throwing monopoly money. These exceptions expose more than most of the other types, in terms of a reflexive and provocative action using their own embodiment and referencing social norms and the respective medium by exaggerating the stereotypical male role or using rather repelling motifs. The third quality of exception counts profiles including rather personal insights into having children (1 of 542), being overweight (2 of 542), being handicapped, having odd hobbies or specific sexual preferences or fetishes (3 of 542). Being resistive towards the social norms of appropriateness and attractiveness in an (online) dating context might be a sign of generalized agency (Holzkamp, 1983 ), for instance of overcoming restrictive social rules and thereby widening one’s scope of action. Although comparatively few pictures belong to these “resistive” types, they are nevertheless significant, since they show the actual possibilities of self-expression, which most people do not use.

Ethics and Limitations

Using data in the form of screen grabbing in social networks should be considered in ethical terms. As Condie, Lean and Wilcockson ( 2017 ) argue, specific consideration is necessary when collecting data in social media. In the social media environment, data is accessible, which does not equal the right to re-use it without protecting personal rights, as data is assumed to always contain personal information (Evans et al., 2015 ). At the same time, research about digital realities/social media is valid and important. The established and published research practices range from simply screen grabbing and publishing content (Abidin, 2016 : Instagram content) to paraphrasing texts so the account cannot be identified by an internet search. For this research design on images, we follow ethics in social media research based on considerations of the association of Internet research, Evans et al ( 2015 ) and Fossheim & Ingierd ( 2015 ). The research is first, not violating the Tinder terms and conditions, which mainly prevent commercialization and re-use of pictures or content for commercials. Second, we did not engage in a relationship with the account holders and we secured the privacy of the data by following the data protection declaration from our university, keeping it strictly confidential. Finally, the pictures presented in the typology are anonymized to a degree that even through a google picture search neither the account, nor the account holder’s digital identity can be revealed, or the person recognized.

Limitations

There are some limitations and specifications to be mentioned for this contribution. The study is relying on data collected on one specific application, namely Tinder; it has yet to be researched whether the logic is generalizable for other mobile online dating applications or if it is Tinder specific. Other applications, like Bumble, discern in both digital architecture, marketing, and the social group of typical users as target group. Furthermore, both series are collected in northern Europe, further research might show to what degree national characteristics play a role. Although one might assume that there are minor differences in the global west, yet there are differing beauty standards and cultural differences. For instance, regarding compliant self-presentation, optimization (e.g., plastic surgery playing different cultural roles in inter alia China, Brazil, US context, and so forth: discussed in Sturm-O’Brien et al., 2010 ) and many more.

In the conduct of research, we collected data taking the role of gender into account, presuming differences, aiming for a balanced sample. In the analysis, the presumptions did not persist in great manner. There are only a few gender differences, many types are gender overarching, for instance the types selfie, informative, and incognito. Gender differences shown in former research for instance regarding sport equipment did not present great relevance for the findings.

This aspect could be focused with regards to simulation effects, where users simply mirror the other’s self-presentation. Users imitate the pictures they see. As the pictures the users see—at least in a heterosexual setting—come from the opposite sex, male and female presentations would align.

When interpreting the findings there are algorithm related aspects to consider: users can choose to let Tinder select their most successful picture out of the possible up to nine profile pictures. Nevertheless, this does not change the significance of the profile pictures, as the algorithm can still only choose from the users’ portfolio. Yet, this aspect could have a streamlining effect as it accelerates the conditioning of specific self-presentation. The reconstructive serial analysis is focused and limited to the intersubjective orientation that again functions as a storyline for behavior—the inherent logic and social rules—it cannot and does not aim for a reconstruction of the individual expectation, motives, or aspired impression/intent nor does it contribute to the individual perception of these forms of self-presentation.

Finally, building a typology contains paradigmatic limitations that should be considered when interpreting the results. When building typologies, the researcher is biased to search for minimum and maximum contrast, resulting in typicality of self-presentation; this does not imply that there is not individuality in the self-presentation and individual nuances.

Conclusion and Outlook: Coming to an Understanding of Selves When Mobile Online Dating

Serial picture analysis in the predominantly visual context of mobile online dating gives valuable insights into implicit social norms and respective subjective referencing in the presentations of the selves, beyond the seemingly obvious. The results show that these norms are oriented towards easily categorizable and countable common motives and presentations, indicating a strong restrictive context and collectively complied norms. The great majority of users orient the self-presentation strongly on these social norms and do not exhaust the vast possibilities of variety, presenting contrary motives or create unique and individual profiles. This goes far beyond the discourse on self-presentation in mobile online dating where picture analysis so far mostly focuses on quantitative analysis of presented motives, colors, and objects and mere speculations about users’ motifs. This contributes by a focus on the actual pictures following the reconstructive logic serving with insights to the users’ implicit knowledge—users know more than they know to know—and can thus reveal more than reflections can access, for instance by asking users about intentions and (post-constructed) reasoning for their self-presentation. The findings add to the current body of research by highlighting a paradox: while the vast majority of users are looking for special moments, for (the) big love, and want to be perceived as someone special, these desires are counteracted by their own self-presentations. This self-assignment to a few types corresponds to an accelerated practice of constant wiping, the result of which—many similar and thus also boring options—they often complain about but produce themselves.

These insights lead to several general questions in the context of social practices and specifically relationship formations. We suggest that subjective behavior is not determined or one-sided causally shaped by the application (Thornham & Cruz, 2015 ). Instead, we assume that digital realities and the established practices have two-sided impacts wherein subjects and technical context reciprocally shape, acquire, and grow with each other (Schraube, 2009 ). One of peoples’ main motives of using mobile online dating applications is finding love and a partner and creating special moments (Degen & Kleeberg-Niepage, 2020 ; Timmermans & DeCaluwe, 2017 ). The analysis directs into a deeper understanding of a tension between motifs of presenting the self to find a fitting partner and to be understood as special and the strive for validation and thus compliant and self-presentation conformity. The established usage of the application seems to establish an intersubjective understanding and increase of a viewing habit conform presentation of the self, possibly due to the accelerated usage of swiping and the orientation towards a quantitative logic that possibly undermines individuality in the presentation. This contributes to the research of online dating by analyzing the actual self-presentation enriching the understanding in the sphere between motives, (high) hopes and expectations and the actual behavior. The practice seems immediately contradictory to the original aim of finding a fitting match. What does the practice of common and somehow unspecific self-presentation mean for user’s ideas of intimate relationships? How does such a self-presentation relate to their self-concept and what does it mean to them personally?

Further research will focus on detailed analyses of single images from our typology on how individual self-presentations are created, leading to a deeper understanding of the presumed significance of age, milieu, class, or gender within these processes.

Abidin, C. (2016). “Aren’t these just young, rich women doing vain things online?”: Influencer selfies as subversive frivolity. Social media + society. Volume: 2 issue: 2, Article first published online: April 11, 2016; Issue published: April 1, 2016.

Bohnsack, R. (2009). Qualitative Bild- und Videointerpretation. Die dokumentarische Methode . Opladen: Budrich.

Google Scholar  

Bohnsack, R. (2010). The interpretation of pictures and the documentary method. In R. Bohnsack, N. Pfaff, & W. Weller, (Eds.), Qualitative analysis and documentary method in international educational research, 9 (3), 267–292.

Bohnsack, R., Michel, B., & Przyborski, A. (2015). Dokumentarische Bildinterpretation. Methodologie und Forschungspraxis . Opladen: Budrich.

Carpenter, C., & McEwan, B. (2016). The players of micro-dating: Individual and gender differences in goal orientations toward micro-dating apps. First Monday, 21 (5).

Condie, J., Lean, G., & Wilcockson, B. (2017). The trouble with Tinder: The ethical complexities of researching location-aware social discovery apps. In K. Woodfield (Ed.), The Ethics of Online Research (Advances in Research Ethics and Integrity, Volume 2) (pp. 135–158). Emerald Publishing: Bingley.

Courtois, C. & Timmermans, E. (2018). Cracking the Tinder code: An experience sampling approach to the dynamics and impact of platform governing algorithms, Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 23 (1) 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmx001 .

Cruz, E. G., & Thornham, H. (2015). Selfies beyond self-representation: the (theoretical) f(r)ictions of a practice. Journal of Aesthetics & Culture, 7 (1), 1–10.

Article   Google Scholar  

Degen, J., & Kleeberg-Niepage, A. (2020). The more we Tinder: Subject, scope, society. Human Arenas . https://doi.org/10.1007/s42087-020-00132-8 .

Duguay, S. (2016a). Dressing up Tinderella: Interrogating authenticity claims on the mobile dating app Tinder. Information, Communication & Society, 20 (3), 351–367.

Duguay, S. (2016b). Lesbian, gay, bisexual, trans, and queer visibility through selfies: Comparing platform mediators across Ruby Rose’s Instagram and Vine presence. Social Media + Society . https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305116641975

Ellison, N., Heino, R., & Gibbs, J. (2006). Managing impressions online: Self-presentation processes in the online dating environment. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 11 (2), 415–441. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00020.x .

Ellison, N. B., Hancock, J. T., & Toma, C. L. (2012). Profile as promise: A framework for conceptualizing veracity in online dating selfpresentations. New media & society, 14 (1), 1–18.

Evans, H., Ginnis, S., & Bartlett, J. (2015). #SocialEthics: aguide to embedding ethics in social media research . Paris: IPSOS.

Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

Fitzpatrick, C., Birnholtz, J., & Brubaker, J. R. (2015). Social and Personal Disclosure in a Location-Based Real Time Dating App. In 48th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences , (pp. 1983–1992). Kauai.

Fossheim, H. & Ingierd, H. (Eds.)(2015).  Internet Research Ethics.  Oslo: Cappelen Damm Akademisk.

Gergen, K. J. (1991). The saturated self: Dilemmas of identity in contemporary life . New York: Basic Books.

Goffman, E. (1959). Wir alle spielen Theater . New York: Doubleday.

Goffman, E. (1981). Forms of Talk . Pennsylvania: Pennsylvania Press.

Hancock, J. T., Toma, C., & Ellison, N. B. (2007). The truth about lying in online dating profiles. In Proceedings of the 2007 Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems , CHI 2007, San Jose, California, USA, April 28 - May 3, 2007.

Harter, S. (2012). Constructing the self . New York: Guilford Press.

Highfield, T., & Leaver, T. (2016). Instagrammatics and digital methods: studying visual social media, from selfies and GIFs to memes and emoji. Communication Research and Practice, 2 (1), 47–62.

Holzkamp, K. (1983). Grundlagen der Psychologie. Frankfurt a.M.: Campus.

Illouz, E. (2007). Cold Intimacies. The making of emotional capitalism . Cambridge: Polity Press.

James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology . New York: Henry Holt.

Litt, Eden (2012). Knock, Knock . Who's There? The Imagined Audience, Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 56 (3), 330–345. https://doi.org/10.1080/08838151.2012.705195

Michel, B. (2015). Orientierungsdilemmata einer korporativen Bildpraxis – Analyse der Vorstandsporträts der Deutschen Bank von 1998–2012. In: R. Bohnsack, M., & A. Przyborski (Eds.). Dokumentarische Bildinterpretation. Methodologie und Forschungspraxis . Opladen: Budrich.

Morse, S., & Gergen, K. J. (1970). Social comparison, self-consistency, and the concept of self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16 (1), 148–156.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Müller-Dohm, S. (1997). Bildinterpretation als struktural-hermeneutische Symbolanalyse. In R. Hitzler (Eds.), Sozialwissenschaftliche Hermeneutik. Eine Einführung , (Vol. 1., pp. 81–108). Wiesbaden: Springer (Universitätstaschenbücher, 1885).

Mummendey, H.-D. (2006). Selbstdarstellung (self-presentation). In H. W. Bierhoff & D. Frey (Eds.), Handbuch der Sozialpsychologie und Kommunikationspsychologie (Reihe Handbuch der Psychologie, Band 3) (pp. 49–56). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Musil, B., Preglej, A., Ropert, T., Klasinc, L., & Babic, N. C. (2017). What is seen is who you are: Are cues in selfie pictures related to personality characteristics? Frontiers in Psychology, 8 (1), 1–13.

Panofsky, E. (1975). Ikonographie & Ikonologie. Eine Einführung in die Kunst der Renaissance. In: Erwin Panofsky: Sinn und Deutung in der Bildenden (pp. 36–38). Kunst. Köln.

Panofsky, E. (1955). Meaning in the visual arts. Papers in and on Art History . New York: Doubleday.

Pilarczyk, U., & Mietzner, U. (2008). Das reflektierte Bild. Die seriell-ikonografische Fotoanalyse in den Erziehungs- und Sozialwissenschaften (Vol. 1.). Bad Heilbrunn: Klinkhardt.

Pleines, C., Bothe, A., & Schweyer, C. (2018). ZU-ZWEIT.de Profilbild-Studie 2017/2018: Partnersuche, Strand und Gurken . In Zu-ZWEIT.de .

Ranzini, G., & Lutz, C. (2017). Love at first swipe? Explaining Tinder self-presentation and motives. Mobile Media & Communication, 5 (1), 80–101.

Reavey, P. (2016). Visual methods in psychology. Using and interpreting images in qualitative research . Abingdon: Routledge.

Rose, N. (2006). Governing the soul: Shaping of the private self . London: Free Association Books.

Statista, 2020. Durchschnittliche Anzahl an Abonnenten von Tinder weltweit vom 1. Quartal 2015 bis zum 1. Quartal 2020. Available: https://de.statista.com/statistik/daten/studie/806485/umfrage/abonnenten-von-tinder-weltweit/

Schraube, E. (2009). Technology as materialized action and its ambivalences. Theory & Psychology, 19 (2), 296–312. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354309103543 .

Schrock, A. R. (2015). Communicative affordances of mobile media: Portability, availability, locatability, and multimediality. International Journal of Communication , 9

Sedgewick, J. R., Flath, M. E., & Elias, L. J. (2017). Presenting your best self(ie): The influence of gender on vertical orientation of selfies on Tinder. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 (1), 1–6.

Sturm-O’Brien, A., Brissett, A. & Brissett, A. (2010). Ethnic trends in facial plastic surgery. Facial Plasict Surgery, 26 (2), 069–074. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0030-1253496 .

Steinke, Ines (2010). Gütekriterien qualitativer Forschung. In Uwe Flick, Ernst von Kardorff & Ines Steinke (Hrsg.), Qualitative Forschung. Ein Handbuch (9th ed., pp. 319–331). Reinbek: Rowohlt.

Timmermans, E., & De Caluwé, E. (2017). Development and validation of the Tinder Motives Scale (TMS). Computers in Human Behavior, 70, 341–350.

Walther, J. B. (2007). Selective self-presentation in computer-mediated communication: Hyperpersonal dimensions of technology, language, and cognition. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 2538–2557.

Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23, 3–44.

Ward, J. (2016). Swiping, Matching, Chatting. Self-Presentation and Self-Disclosure on Mobile Dating Apps. In Human IT: Journal for Information Technology Studies as a Human Science, 13 (2), 81–95.

Weigel, M. (2016). Labor of love: The invention of dating . New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

European University of Flensburg, Flensburg, Germany

Johanna Lisa Degen & Andrea Kleeberg-Niepage

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Johanna Lisa Degen .

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Degen, J.L., Kleeberg-Niepage, A. Profiling the Self in Mobile Online Dating Apps: a Serial Picture Analysis. Hu Arenas 6 , 147–171 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s42087-021-00195-1

Download citation

Received : 09 October 2020

Revised : 27 January 2021

Accepted : 29 January 2021

Published : 13 February 2021

Issue Date : March 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s42087-021-00195-1

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Picture Analysis
  • Mobile online dating
  • Profiling the self
  • Profile picture

Advertisement

  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

We've detected unusual activity from your computer network

To continue, please click the box below to let us know you're not a robot.

Why did this happen?

Please make sure your browser supports JavaScript and cookies and that you are not blocking them from loading. For more information you can review our Terms of Service and Cookie Policy .

For inquiries related to this message please contact our support team and provide the reference ID below.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Int J Environ Res Public Health

Logo of ijerph

Relationships between Social Networking Sites Use and Self-Esteem: The Moderating Role of Gender

Associated data.

Data are available from the author upon request.

With the prevalence of the internet, there is growing attention on the impacts of social networking sites use among adolescents. The purpose of this study was to explore the relationships between different types of online activities (i.e., information searching, social interaction and entertainment) and self-esteem. It examined whether the relationships vary across gender. One hundred and ninety-three students (57.5% males; M age = 13.33, SD age = 1.58) participated in the present study. Unexpectedly, the associations between online activities and self-esteem were not significant ( p > 0.05). Path analysis showed gender moderated the relationships between social interaction activities and self-esteem. Females reported higher levels of engagement in social interaction activities and self-esteem than their male counterparts. The present study shows the importance of assessing different types of online activities as a predictor for understanding the impact of social media use among adolescents.

1. Introduction

With the prevalence of the internet and smartphone, social networking sites (SNSs) have been increasingly popular over the past decades. As of July 2022, over five billion people use the internet; while nearly 4.7 billion are active social media users [ 1 ]. Compared to other countries, Asian people were most frequent social media users (over 2 billion) [ 2 ]. In this context, Facebook is the most frequent use SNS (almost 3 billion), followed by YouTube (almost 2.5 billion) and Instagram (almost 1.5 billion) [ 3 ].

Given the increased popularity of the SNSs, scholars began to explore the potential impacts of SNS use. Excessive SNS use is defined as “being overly concerned about social media, driven by an uncontrollable motivation to log on to or use social media, and devoting so much time and effort to social media that it impairs other important life areas” ([ 4 ], p. 4054). A review study suggested that SNS use may pose threats to adolescent mental health, such as depression, anxiety, poor academic performance, sleep deprivation and other behavioral problems, [ 5 ]. In particular, one prominent line of research explores the impact of SNS use on self-esteem.

Self-esteem is described as an individual’s overall evaluation of his or her own worth [ 6 ]. A growing body of research suggested that self-esteem is an important predictor of well-being [ 7 , 8 ] and life satisfaction [ 9 ]. The impact of self-esteem on well-being is further supported in meta-analyses [ 10 , 11 ].

As a part of self-concept [ 12 ], researchers began to explore the role of self-esteem on SNS use [ 13 , 14 ]. First, the linkage between SNS use and self-esteem is associated through the cognitive process [ 15 ]. Individual’s self-esteem might be enhanced when they received “ likes ” on their posted content [ 16 ] or developed a sense of connectedness through interpersonal communication [ 17 , 18 , 19 ]. Second, adolescents with emotional and adjustment problems are likely to engage in SNS as a self-soothing experience to escape from reality [ 20 , 21 ]. Individuals, especially those with low self-esteem, can benefit from this medium which allows them to socialize comfortably and seek social recognition [ 22 , 23 ]. The evidence of developing social capital among low self-esteem individual has been support by a recent meta-analysis [ 24 ]. This has been further demonstrated by an experimental study [ 25 ] which showed that SNS use (i.e., Facebook ) is associated with a short-term increase in self-esteem.

While SNS use becomes an essential part of our life, scholars start to test the impact of SNS use. Past studies have shown that SNS use is a significant predictor of poor well-being [ 20 , 26 ]. For example, the use of SNS (e.g., Facebook ) is negatively associated with self-esteem among young adults [ 13 , 27 , 28 ]. Using a four-year cross-lagged longitudinal design, Steinsbekk et al. [ 29 ] found that passive SNS use (e.g., browsing others’ profile) is negatively predicted self-esteem among a sample of Norwegian adolescents. Notably, this relationship is more salient among adolescent girls who frequently engage in social comparison on social media.

Although the negative impacts of SNS use on self-esteem have been found, researchers argue that SNS use maybe beneficial to well-being [ 30 ]. Studies found that SNS use is associated with better subjective well-being (e.g., life satisfaction) and less depressive symptoms and loneliness [ 31 , 32 ]. For instance, Jelenchick et al. [ 33 ] found the benefits of SNS use for patients with depressive symptoms. Similar results are shown in self-esteem. Researchers showed that intense SNS use is associated with higher self-esteem [ 34 , 35 ]. Furthermore, cross-sectional [ 36 , 37 ] and experimental studies [ 24 , 38 ] suggested that SNS use is associated with momentary positive changes in self-esteem. In particular, a weak and negative effect of SNS use on social self-esteem, ranging from −04 to −09 has consistently been found in meta-analytic work [ 25 , 39 ]. Taken together, the aforementioned studies showed the relations between SNS use and well-being remains unclear. Researchers noted that the prevalence of a cross-sectional design may limit our understanding of the temporal relationships among variables in the field of SNS use research [ 39 , 40 , 41 ].

Perhaps, the mixed results may indicate the limitations of the existing literature. First, the available findings have exclusively focused on a specific social networking site, such as Facebook [ 42 , 43 , 44 ], Instagram [ 45 ] or general social media use [ 46 ]. Researchers noted that the well-being impact of SNS use may vary depending on the motives and content of the SNS use [ 7 , 46 ]. This is supported by Carlson et al. [ 47 ] who suggested that people engage in SNS use mainly for three different purposes, such as information searching, entertainment, and social interaction. To date, little is known whether the impact of SNS use varies by different types of online activity. Researchers call for the need to assess the consequences of different types of online activities in order to comprehensively capture the whole array of SNS use in this area of research [ 29 , 48 ].

Moreover, studies have mostly focused on the relationships between SNS use and psychological vulnerabilities. Researchers argued the need to take other potential confounders into account [ 49 , 50 ]. Past studies have controlled socio-demographic factors as covariates, such as age, gender, and socioeconomic status, but little has explored the moderating effect of these variables. Researchers noted that investigating the effects of these variables may extend the existing literature [ 51 , 52 ]. The present study attempts to fill this research gap by testing the moderating role of gender.

Past studies show that gender may moderate the relationships between internet addiction and psychological outcomes [ 53 , 54 ]. Adolescents are likely to use the social media platforms to gain social support and to express their negative emotions freely with other users [ 55 ]. Compared to males, females’ self-esteem is more social-oriented [ 56 ]. Females are more likely to develop social media addictive symptoms [ 57 ] and actively engage in online social interaction activities [ 22 ]; while males are more inclined to participate in solitary activities, such as online gaming [ 58 , 59 ]. It is noteworthy that these associations were more prevalent in younger social media users compared to their older counterparts [ 60 ]. Individuals with emotional and adjustment problems are sensitive to interpersonal relationships and therefore may prefer to socialize in virtual contexts in which they feel less threatened [ 26 , 61 , 62 ]. To date, little is known about the moderating role of gender underlying these relations. Hence, the present study investigates whether the relationships between different types of SNS use and self-esteem differ by gender.

To summarize, the present study aimed to extend the literature by exploring the relationships between different types of online activities (i.e., information searching, social interaction and entertainment) and self-esteem. Moreover, it tested whether these relationships vary across gender. The present study attempts to address the following research questions:

RQ 1: Does SNS use relate to self-esteem? If so, how different types of online activity (i.e., information searching, social interaction and entertainment) are related to self-esteem? Based on the literature, it appears that all online activities will have negative effects on self-esteem.

RQ2: Does gender moderate the relationship between SNS use and self-esteem?

Compared to males, the potential effects of online activities on self-esteem will be much stronger among females.

A conceptual model of the present study is shown in Figure 1 .

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is ijerph-19-11462-g001.jpg

A hypothesized model of the present study.

2.1. Participants

193 students (111 males = 57.7%, 1 student did not report his/her gender information) from two secondary schools in Hong Kong participated ( Table 1 ). These schools were recruited through a service-learning project which aimed at addressing community-identified needs by providing meaningful services activities [ 63 ]. The mean age is 13.33 ( SD = 1.58), ranging from 12 to 19 years old (males: M age = 13.53, SD age = 1.67; female: M age =13.05, SD age = 1.43). Sixty-one percent of the participants received financial aid either from their schools or the government ( n = 116, 2 students did not report this information). One hundred and fifty-nine students (83.2%) were born in Hong Kong. One hundred and thirty-one students (69.7%) were from intact families. The study was conducted in Spring–Summer 2019.

Demographic background of the participants.

WholeMale
( = 111, 57.5%)
Female
( = 81, 42.5%)
Age = 13.33, = 1.58 = 13.53,
= 1.67
= 13.05,
= 1.43
= 2.15, = 185.02, < 0.05
Place of birth (1) = 1.03, =1, >.05
Hong Kong159 (83.2%)94 (84.7%)64 (79%)
Others33 (16.8%)17 (15.3%)17 (21%)
Receiving financial aids (1) = 0.06, = 1, > 0.05
Yes116 (60.7%)69 (62.2%)49 (60.5%)
No75 (39.3%)42 (37.8%)32 (39.5%)
Family structure (1) = 0.01, = 1, > 0.05
Intact131 (69.7%)76 (68.5%)55 (67.9%)
Non-intact62 (30.3%)35 (31.5%)26 (32.1%)

Note . a Received financial aid from school or government.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. online activities.

Following the findings of Carlson’s study [ 47 ], online activities were assessed by eight items focusing on three areas: information searching (two items, “google search”, “chatroom”, Cronbach’s alpha = 0.67), entertainment (four items, “watching YouTube ”, “download software”, “online gaming”, “online shopping”, Cronbach’s alpha = 0.70), social interaction (two items, “social media platforms”, such as “ Instagram , Facebook , Snapchat ” and communication, such as “email, WhatsApp , WeChat ”, Cronbach’s alpha = 0.75). Participants rated on a five-point scale, ranging from 0 (never) to 5 (more than seven times per week), with high scores indicating high levels of engagement in a specific type of online activities.

2.2.2. Self-Esteem

The 10-items Rosenberg self-esteem scale [ 64 ] was adopted to measure students’ perceived self-esteem. Participants rated on a four-point scale, ranging from 1 “strongly disagree” to 4 “strongly agree”. A higher overall score indicates a higher level of self-esteem. An example item “I take a positive attitude toward myself”. The present sample showed an acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.78).

2.3. Procedures

This study was approved by the university’s Institutional Review Board. Students anonymously completed a paper-and-pencil-based questionnaire with demographic information (e.g., age, gender, place of birth, receiving financial aids) after obtaining the informed consent from the school principals, teachers and parents. In general, it took around 15 min to complete the questionnaires.

2.4. Data Analysis

Descriptive statistics and correlations among the main variables were computed via IBM SPSS 28.0 version. Independent t -test and chi-square preliminary test were performed to assess the relationship between independent variables and dependent variables. Then, path analysis was conducted to investigate the moderating role of gender on the relationships between online activities and self-esteem using Mplus 8.7 version. Past studies [ 65 , 66 , 67 , 68 ] showed the effects of age and socio-economic status on SNS use, therefore they were included in the analyses as covariate. Missing data were less than 1%. Prior to path analysis, normality of all observed variables is assessed. Results showed that the data are normally distributed (skewness and kurtosis between −2.0 to 2.0) [ 69 ], therefore maximum likelihood (ML) was used to estimate the model ( Table 2 ). To evaluate the model fit, several indices were used, including the Chi-square values ( χ 2 ), the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis fit index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and the standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR). Researchers noted that CFI and TLI above 0.90 and RMSEA and SRMR below 0.08 indicated a good model fit [ 70 ].

Descriptive statistics and correlations of all variables.

Variable SkewnessKurtosisα1234
1. Information searching2.431.350.118−0.8440.67-
2. Social interaction3.641.51−0.780−0.6620.750.54 **-
3. Entertainment2.451.060.034−0.1560.700.54 **0.42 **-
4. Self-esteem2.710.450.1980.1140.780.040.07−0.01-

** p < 0.01.

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

Results of chi-square tests of independence and independent t -test showed no significant gender differences regarding their demographic and family background, except in age ( p < 0.05, Table 1 ). Descriptive statistics and correlation coefficient among the variables were shown in Table 2 . First, all online activities were moderately related, ranging from 0.42 to 0.54. Second, overall internet use and different types of online activities were not significantly related to self-esteem ( Table 2 ). Results of independent t -test showed no significant gender differences in all variables, except in entertainment online activities ( p < 0.05, Table 3 ). This indicates females were more likely to use social media for entertainment purpose than males while both genders shared similar usage patterns in other online activities.

Gender differences among all variables.

VariableMaleFemale Cohen’s
Information searching2.451.352.411.370.191690.03
Social interaction3.471.483.851.54−1.64169−0.25
Entertainment2.600.982.251.162.17 *1530.33
Self-esteem2.730.462.670.420.941740.14

* p < 0.05.

3.2. Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed to test the factor structure of the measures. The factor structure of three types of online activities ( χ 2 = 34.677, df = 16, p < 0.01; RMSEA = 0.079 (CI = 0.042–0.115); SRMR = 0.051; TLI = 0.905; CFI = 0.946) and self-esteem ( χ 2 = 44.69, df = 15, p < 0.01; RMSEA = 0.069 (CI = 0.026–0.108); SRMR = 0.046; TLI = 0.941; CFI = 0.968) fit the data well. The reliability of one of the online activities (information searching) was somewhat low (α = 0.67) but can still be considered as acceptable (Cronbach α = 0.70) [ 71 ]. In general, the reliability of both scales reached at an adequate level, ranging from 0.67 to 0.78.

3.3. The Moderating Role of Gender

To test the moderating role of gender on the relations between online activities on self-esteem, path analysis was conducted. Following the suggestions by Dearing and Hamilton [ 72 ], all predictors were standardized to reduce the chance of multicollinearity. Gender ( β = −0.52, p < 0.01) and its interaction effect with social interaction ( β = 0.53, p < 0.01) were significantly related to self-esteem when controlling for age and socioeconomic status ( χ 2 = 0.00, df = 0, p < 0.01; CFI = 1.0; TLI = 1.0; SRMR = 0.00; RMSEA = 0.00). All antecedent variables explained 13% of the variance in self-esteem, suggesting that three different types of online activity and gender explained 13% of the variance in self-esteem ( Table 4 ).

Standardized coefficients of path analysis.

Estimate
Gender → self-esteem−0.52 **0.17
Information searching → self-esteem−0.060.11
Social interaction → self-esteem0.040.11
Entertainment → self-esteem0.040.12
Information searching X Gender → self-esteem−0.050.19
Social interaction X Gender → self-esteem0.53 **0.20
Entertainment X Gender → self-esteem−0.110.22
0.13 **

3.4. Analyses of Covariance (ANCOVAs)

Three univariate analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were performed to test the differences in online activities among participants of different demographic (i.e., gender) and psychological characteristics (i.e., self-esteem) while controlling for age and socioeconomic status. Results of ANCOVAs revealed a significant gender difference in the relation between social interaction use of SNS and self-esteem, F (1,1818) = 4.385, p = 0.04, η2 = 0.02). The effect size of this difference was small [ 73 ]. While no significant difference was found in other online activities (information searching: F (1,1178) = 0.414, p > 0.05; entertainment: F (1,1180) = 2.284, p > 0.05). Results demonstrated that females were likely to engage in social interaction activities and reported high self-esteem; while males reported lower levels of engagement in social interaction activities and self-esteem ( Figure 2 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is ijerph-19-11462-g002.jpg

The moderating effect of gender on the relationship between social interaction of SNS use and self-esteem.

4. Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to test the moderating role of gender on the relationship between SNS use and self-esteem. First, a significant interaction effect of gender X SNS on self-esteem was found. Specifically, females were more likely to engage in online social interaction activities and reported higher self-esteem compared to their male counterparts. This was consistent with the past findings consistently showed that females were more addictive to social media use [ 74 , 75 ] and gain online social popularities [ 76 ] than males. Empirical evidence shows that social motive was a significant predictor to social media addiction [ 77 ]. Perhaps, females may benefit from SNS use with the intention to satisfy their social needs, and thus reported higher self-esteem. This is in line with research showing the positive outcomes of the SNSs use, such as psychological well-being [ 78 , 79 ] and academic experiences [ 80 ]. Clearly, future research should explore whether satisfaction of social needs is associated with SNSs use, and how this relation differs by gender and other predisposing factors, e.g., personality, social skills.

Contrary to the past research, the relations between different online activities and self-esteem are not significant. This might be related to students’ characteristics. The present study involved students who were mostly from low school banding (i.e., students with low academic abilities) [ 81 ]. In particular, they were from low socio-economic status families (over 60% received financial aids) and studied in the two districts, Tuen Mun and Yuen Long, with the highest poverty rates [ 82 ]. Past studies show the influence of socio-economic status on the linkage between the SNS use and well-being [ 52 , 83 ] and self-esteem [ 41 , 67 ]. Perhaps, their family background may be a stronger predictor of self-esteem compared to SNS use. Given the prevalence of SNS use, researchers argued that other potential factors (e.g., online social comparison, personality, nature of SNS use) may be more influential in predicting individual well-being [ 18 , 84 , 85 ]. Future research should investigate how these factors moderate the relationships between online activities and self-esteem.

Another explanation of these unexpected results may be related to the social network size. Lim et al. [ 86 ] found that the negative effects of SNS use on self-esteem was significant only when social network size (less than 150 individual) was small. Young adults and adolescents are frequent SNS users who consider this as part of their daily life. As such, it is not surprise they have a larger social network compare to other population group. In particular, they might not be able to distinguish between “ real ” and “ virtual ” friendship [ 87 ]. This has been confirmed by Apaolaza et al. [ 88 ] who found that the effects of SNS use on social self-esteem were mediated by the quality of online interpersonal relationships among a sample of Spanish adolescents. Future research may explore the role of social network size regarding the relationship between SNS use and self-esteem.

The present study extends the literature by showing the moderating role of gender on the relationships between online activities and self-esteem. It demonstrates the impact of online activities may depend on “gender”, despite its small effect as shown in the present study. This contributes to the notion on the well-being effects of internet use by considering the participants’ demographic background (e.g., gender). As the internet can provide access to information and social resources, students are likely to use this medium to build social capital, satisfy their social needs and compensate their real-life social relationships by gaining positive feedback and popularity in this virtual context [ 22 , 89 ]. Educators may need to pay more attention to this group who are more susceptible to peer pressure [ 90 ] and use social media to receive social validation [ 45 ].

One uniqueness of the present study is that it measured different types of online activities. This certainly serve as a positive response to the researchers’ call for the need to capture various online activities and its impacts on adolescent well-being [ 30 ]. Intervention programs targeting the proper use of SNS among adolescents can be noted in the present study. Educational effort should be implemented for promoting internet literacy, such as negative and positive communication on SNS platforms.

The present study extends the literature by showing the moderating role of gender on the relation between SNS use and self-esteem. Nowadays, adolescent consider SNS as a salient source to obtain social attention and support [ 91 ]. They tend to gravitate towards SNS in order to constantly stay connected with their “ friends ”. It is noteworthy that different SNS use explain a moderate amount of variance (i.e., 13%) in self-esteem. This is consistent with past work by Hawi and Samaha [ 92 ] who compared gender differences in the relationships between SNSs use and self-esteem (below 10%) among university students in Lebanon. To capture a deeper understanding of mechanism between SNS use and well-being, Saiphoo et al. [ 24 ] argued that “ Frequency of SNS use may not be a nuanced enough measure ” (p. 9). Future investigation may explore how these relations differ by the nature of SNS use. Past research found that the impact of SNS use depends on whether individuals are actively or passively engaging with social media content [ 93 , 94 ]. Additionally, more research is warranted to explore how other factors, such as fear of missing out [ 95 ] and effortful control [ 13 ], are associated with SNS use and self-esteem.

The present study has some limitations. First, a cross-sectional design has been adopted. Therefore, causal inferences about the linkage between online activities and well-being cannot be inferred. Future research may employ experimental or longitudinal design to demonstrate causal evidence. Second, the sample size of the present study was relatively modest ( N = 193). Yet, the findings were robust as the fit statistics show satisfactory results. Third, findings were based on self-report measures, which might be influenced by social desirability, although confidentiality and anonymity have been highlighted during the data collection process. Lastly, the data were collected based on adolescents. Findings may not be generalized to other populations (e.g., adults, elderly).

5. Conclusions

The present study demonstrates how gender moderates the relation between online activities and well-being. Given the prevalence of the SNSs in our daily life, more research in this area is warranted to help us understand how SNSs relate to health and psychological well-being.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to express her sincere gratitude to the schools, teachers and students who participated in the study.

Funding Statement

This research was funded by the Quality Education Fund of the Education Bureau, Hong Kong SAR (under project number: EDB/QEF/2017/0441).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by the institutional review board of The Hong Kong Polytechnic University (approval number: HSEARS20181105002).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from school, parents and participants.

Data Availability Statement

Conflicts of interest.

The author declares no conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

IMAGES

  1. ≫ Online Dating Apps and Self-Esteem Free Essay Sample on Samploon.com

    online dating and self esteem research paper

  2. (PDF) Selfie and Self-Esteem

    online dating and self esteem research paper

  3. Digital Self-Esteem Journal by International Teaching Nomad

    online dating and self esteem research paper

  4. Key findings about online dating in the U.S.

    online dating and self esteem research paper

  5. Nature of Online Dating Research Paper Example

    online dating and self esteem research paper

  6. 📌 Self-esteem Research Essay Example

    online dating and self esteem research paper

VIDEO

  1. More about me on my channel🙀How do men manipulate you? How to get all you want from men? #dating

  2. GRWM as if we’re on FaceTime

  3. Avoidant Personality Disorder & being worthy of love

  4. Dove

  5. Teen Dating & Healthy Relationships in the Digital Age Webinar

  6. Exploring the Desire for Positive Ownership: Why Some Women Seek It

COMMENTS

  1. Dating Apps and Their Sociodemographic and Psychosocial Correlates: A Systematic Review

    1. Introduction. In the last decade, the popularization of the Internet and the use of the smartphone and the emergence of real-time location-based dating apps (e.g., Tinder, Grindr) have transformed traditional pathways of socialization and promoted new ways of meeting and relating to potential romantic and/or sexual partners [1,2,3,4].It is difficult to know reliably how many users currently ...

  2. Online Dating and Problematic Use: A Systematic Review

    Sex-search and self-esteem enhancement are predictors of problematic use of online dating. Previous research coincides with online dating risks (e.g. fear of deception) and objectification tendency due to online dating services (sites and apps) design. Observations regarding methodological weaknesses and future research implications are included.

  3. (PDF) Self-Esteem in Online Dating

    Self-esteem is defined by Rosenberg, (1965) as "the positive or negative attitudes that a person has about himself.". In a study by Kim, Kwon and Lee (2009), they evaluated three characteristics of users of online dating platforms, namely: self-esteem, involvement in romantic 125 frelationships and sociability.

  4. (PDF) The Role of Self-Esteem on the Effects of Motivations of Online

    self-esteem or se lf-worth suffering negatively, as a result of online dating. The purpose of this study will serve as additional information and research in a field that is ste adily growing in ...

  5. (PDF) Relationship between online dating app usage, self-esteem, and

    self-esteem score of 9.24, while their body image score was 10.05. On the other hand, non-dating app users scored an average of 7.41 in self-esteem and 10.28 in body image. These results suggest ...

  6. Online dating and problematic use: A systematic review.

    Sex-search and self-esteem enhancement are predictors of problematic use of online dating. Previous research coincides with online dating risks (e.g. fear of deception) and objectification tendency due to online dating services (sites and apps) design. Observations regarding methodological weaknesses and future research implications are included.

  7. Online dating and psychological wellbeing: A social compensation

    Introduction. Online dating refers to the use of websites and mobile applications for finding romantic partners for short- and long-term relationships [1].Having started as a niche practice in the 1990's, online dating has now entered the mainstream [2], recently becoming the most popular venue for meeting relationship partners in the U.S [3].As of 2020, 30% of Americans had used a dating ...

  8. PDF Online Dating and Problematic Use: A Systematic Review

    atic use of online dating, resulting in 43 studies. Findings suggest that personality correlates such as neuroticism, sociability, sensation-seeking, and sexual permissiveness are related to greater use of online dating services. Sex-search and self-esteem enhance-ment are predictors of problematic use of online dating. Previous research ...

  9. Tinder Users: Sociodemographic, Psychological, and Psychosexual

    1. Introduction. The development of online dating has grown considerably in recent years and has changed the way some people interact with potential new romantic and/or sexual partners, especially after the emergence and popularization of dating apps [].In fact, different studies have shown that around 40% of single adults are looking for an online partner [] or that about 25% of new couples ...

  10. PDF Online Dating Applications and The Impact on Well-being 1

    DekkersDr. P.M. ten KloosterJanuary 25th, 2021AbstractOnline da. ing has become increasing. y popular in today's society. However, its positive andnegative impact on use. s' well-bein. current cross-sectional study aimed to examine if the frequency of usage of online dating. g, with low frequency usage assumed to be associated withhig.

  11. PDF The Relationship between Online Dating, Self-Esteem and Body Image

    the fact that self-esteem has a crucial role in determining partner choices offline, little is known about the role self-esteem has when it comes to online dating. Therefore, the current paper focuses on online dating with a specific focus on self-esteem and body image. In the

  12. Self-Disclosure and Self-Efficacy in Online Dating

    Paper 889. This study explores online dating by studying the relationship between self-disclosure and self-efficacy in an online dating environment. This research study examines the way self-disclosure, self-efficacy, self-esteem, and gender interrelate in an online dating environment. This study includes, but is not limited to, discussion of ...

  13. Dating App Use and Wellbeing: An Application-Based Pilot Study

    2.1. Design. The study consisted of real-time self-reported repeated measures collected using a newly developed smartphone app (i.e., DiaryMood) in which participants responded to questions regarding the following areas three times a day: (i) mood, (ii) self-esteem, and (iii) craving (i.e., in the morning, afternoon, and evening).In addition, participants included their daily use of dating ...

  14. PDF Online Dating: A Critical Analysis From the

    Research Article Summary Online dating sites frequently claim that they have fundamentally altered the dating landscape for the better. This article employs psychological science to examine (a) whether online dating is fundamentally different from conventional offline dating and (b) whether online dating promotes better romantic outcomes than ...

  15. The psychological influence of dating app matches: The more matches the

    Future research using a dating app paradigm could also experiment with the dosage of matches. Since a single intervention could have been too weak to affect state loneliness, future research should repeatedly provide dating app feedback. Prolonged exposure would also allow researchers to observe how participants cope with peer feedback over time.

  16. Dating gone mobile: Demographic and personality-based correlates of

    While our understanding of mobile dating is growing, this body of research has at least three limitations. First, with the exception of the study of the Pew Internet Research Center (Smith, 2016) among 2001 US adults, the studies in this area have used convenience samples.Second, the majority of studies has not specifically looked at young adulthood as a key developmental stage to understand ...

  17. Swipe-based dating applications use and its association with mental

    Swipe-Based Dating Applications (SBDAs) function similarly to other social media and online dating platforms but have the unique feature of "swiping" the screen to either like or dislike another user's profile. There is a lack of research into the relationship between SBDAs and mental health outcomes. The aim of this study was to study whether adult SBDA users report higher levels of ...

  18. Research Paper Gender differences in the relationship between

    Online Deception is growing as a topic of research due to its potential risks for mental health and behaviors. This study explored the relationships between attachment styles and intentional misrepresentation of oneself in online dating (i.e., online deception) and investigated the mediating role of self-esteem.

  19. PDF Swipe-based dating applications use and its association with mental

    0.18 or lower for self-esteem and the scores for app usage, dating behaviour and tinder intensity [13]. A study by Tran et al. of almost 1800 adults found that dating application users were significantly more likely to engage in unhealthy weight control behaviours (such as laxative use, self-induced vomiting and use of anabolic

  20. Studying the Relationship of Problematic Online Dating ...

    The aim of this study was to investigate the connection of problematic online dating (POD), problematic social media (PSM), and problematic online sexual behaviors (POSB), with body esteem and sexuality. Previous research focused either on the impact of media on body esteem, or the impact of body esteem on sexuality. Yet, online media tends to be sexually self-objectifying, requiring more ...

  21. Online dating: A motivated behavior during pandemic

    Abstract. Online dating received a recent upsurge since the outbreak of pandemic with most people confining themselves to virtual dating. This paper conceptualizes and draws conclusion from the ...

  22. Profiling the Self in Mobile Online Dating Apps: a Serial Picture

    Profiles in the widely used phenomenon of mobile online dating applications are characteristically reduced to condensed information mostly containing one or a few pictures. Thus, these picture(s) play a significant role for the decision-making processes and success, supposedly holding vital meaning for the subjects. While profile pictures in social media are omnipresent and some research has ...

  23. Online Dating Caused a Rise in US Income Inequality, Research Paper

    Online dating may be partially to blame for an increase in income inequality in the US in recent decades, according to a research paper.. Since the emergence of dating apps that allow people to ...

  24. Relationships between Social Networking Sites Use and Self-Esteem: The

    Self-esteem is described as an individual's overall evaluation of his or her own worth . A growing body of research suggested that self-esteem is an important predictor of well-being [7,8] and life satisfaction . The impact of self-esteem on well-being is further supported in meta-analyses [10,11].

  25. The Psychological Tapestry of Orthorexia Nervosa: Unpacking its

    Purposive sampling was employed for selecting 274 young individuals to participate in this cross-sectional research. Data was collected from an online survey using Test of Orthorexia Nervosa-17, generalized anxiety disorder-7, Rosenberg self-esteem inventory, and eating attitude test-26.